Research Studies
Study Tracker
Fluoride Exposure and Children’s IQ Scores: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis.Abstract
Key Points
Question Is fluoride exposure associated with children’s IQ scores?
Findings Despite differences in exposure and outcome measures and risk of bias across studies, and when using group-level and individual-level exposure estimates, this systematic review and meta-analysis of 74 cross-sectional and prospective cohort studies found significant inverse associations between fluoride exposure and children’s IQ scores. For fluoride measured in water, associations remained inverse when exposed groups were restricted to less than 4 mg/L or less than 2 mg/L but not when restricted to less than 1.5 mg/L; for fluoride measured in urine, associations remained inverse at less than 4 mg/L, less than 2 mg/L, and less than 1.5 mg/L; and among the subset of low risk-of-bias studies, there were inverse associations when exposed groups were restricted to less than 4 mg/L, less than 2 mg/L, and less than 1.5 mg/L for analyses of fluoride measured both in water and in urine.
Meaning This comprehensive meta-analysis may inform future risk-benefit assessments of the use of fluoride in children’s oral health.
Importance Previous meta-analyses suggest that fluoride exposure is adversely associated with children’s IQ scores. An individual’s total fluoride exposure comes primarily from fluoride in drinking water, food, and beverages.
Objective To perform a systematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiological studies investigating children’s IQ scores and prenatal or postnatal fluoride exposure.
Data Sources BIOSIS, Embase, PsycInfo, PubMed, Scopus, Web of Science, CNKI, and Wanfang, searched through October 2023.
Study Selection Studies reporting children’s IQ scores, fluoride exposure, and effect sizes.
Data Extraction and Synthesis Data were extracted into the Health Assessment Workplace Collaborative system. Study quality was evaluated using the OHAT risk-of-bias tool. Pooled standardized mean differences (SMDs) and regression coefficients were estimated with random-effects models.
Main Outcomes and Measures Children’s IQ scores.
Results Of 74 studies included (64 cross-sectional and 10 cohort studies), most were conducted in China (n?=?45); other locations included Canada (n?=?3), Denmark (n?=?1), India (n?=?12), Iran (n?=?4), Mexico (n?=?4), New Zealand (n?=?1), Pakistan (n?=?2), Spain (n?=?1), and Taiwan (n?=?1). Fifty-two studies were rated high risk of bias and 22 were rated low risk of bias. Sixty-four studies reported inverse associations between fluoride exposure measures and children’s IQ. Analysis of 59 studies with group-level measures of fluoride in drinking water, dental fluorosis, or other measures of fluoride exposure (47 high risk of bias, 12 low risk of bias; n?=?20?932 children) showed an inverse association between fluoride exposure and IQ (pooled SMD, ?0.45; 95% CI, ?0.57 to ?0.33; P?<?.001). In 31 studies reporting fluoride measured in drinking water, a dose-response association was found between exposed and reference groups (SMD, ?0.15; 95% CI, ?0.20 to ?0.11; P?<?.001), and associations remained inverse when exposed groups were restricted to less than 4 mg/L and less than 2 mg/L; however, the association was null at less than 1.5 mg/L. In analyses restricted to low risk-of-bias studies, the association remained inverse when exposure was restricted to less than 4 mg/L, less than 2 mg/L, and less than 1.5 mg/L fluoride in drinking water. In 20 studies reporting fluoride measured in urine, there was an inverse dose-response association (SMD, ?0.15; 95% CI, ?0.23 to ?0.07; P?<?.001). Associations remained inverse when exposed groups were restricted to less than 4 mg/L, less than 2 mg/L, and less than 1.5 mg/L fluoride in urine; the associations held in analyses restricted to the low risk-of-bias studies. Analysis of 13 studies with individual-level measures found an IQ score decrease of 1.63 points (95% CI, ?2.33 to ?0.93; P?<?.001) per 1-mg/L increase in urinary fluoride. Among low risk-of-bias studies, there was an IQ score decrease of 1.14 points (95% CI, –1.68 to –0.61; P?<?.001). Associations remained inverse when stratified by risk of bias, sex, age, outcome assessment type, country, exposure timing, and exposure matrix.
Conclusions and Relevance This systematic review and meta-analysis found inverse associations and a dose-response association between fluoride measurements in urine and drinking water and children’s IQ across the large multicountry epidemiological literature. There were limited data and uncertainty in the dose-response association between fluoride exposure and children’s IQ when fluoride exposure was estimated by drinking water alone at concentrations less than 1.5 mg/L. These findings may inform future comprehensive public health risk-benefit assessments of fluoride exposures.
Fluoride from natural sources occurs in some community water systems (CWSs), and in the United States and some other countries, fluoride is added to public drinking water systems or salt for the prevention of tooth decay. For CWSs that add fluoride, the US Public Health Service recommends a fluoride concentration of 0.7 mg/L, the US Environmental Protection Agency’s (EPA’s) enforceable and nonenforceable standards for fluoride in drinking water are 4.0 mg/L and 2.0 mg/L,1 and the World Health Organization’s (WHO’s) drinking water quality guideline for fluoride is 1.5 mg/L.2 Water and water-based beverages are the main source of systemic fluoride intake. In the United States, the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) estimates that water and processed beverages (eg, soda and juices) provide approximately 75% of a person’s fluoride intake,3 and EPA estimates that 40% to 70% of a person’s fluoride intake comes from fluoridated drinking water.4 However, an individual’s total exposure also reflects contributions from fluoride in other sources, such as food, dental products, industrial emissions, and pharmaceuticals.4 Accumulating evidence suggests that fluoride exposure may affect brain development. A 2006 report from the National Research Council (NRC) concluded that high levels of naturally occurring fluoride in drinking water may be of concern for neurotoxic effects.5 This finding was largely based on studies from endemic fluorosis areas in China that had limitations in study design or methods. Following the NRC review, studies from an additional 10 countries have been published (eFigure 1A in Supplement 1). Previous meta-analyses6–8 found an inverse association between fluoride exposure and children’s IQ. Since the most recent meta-analysis,8 4 new studies on exposure to fluoride and children’s IQ have been published, including 3 studies9–11 that measured individual-level maternal and children’s urinary fluoride concentrations.
To incorporate newer evidence and increase transparency, objectivity, and rigor in the analysis of fluoride research, we conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis of studies that provided estimates of group-level and individual-level fluoride exposure in relation to children’s IQ scores.
The search, selection, extraction, and risk-of-bias evaluation of studies were part of a larger systematic review.12 Brief methods are outlined herein, with detailed methods available in the protocol13 and the “Detailed Methods” section of eAppendix 1 in Supplement 1. This study follows the Meta-Analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) reporting guidelines. Data analysis was conducted from June 2020 to January 2024. The most recent analysis update was performed in January and February 2024.
Literature searches were conducted in BIOSIS, Embase, PsycInfo, PubMed, Scopus, Web of Science, CNKI, and Wanfang. The searches were performed through October 2023 without language restrictions.13 Studies were independently screened by 2 reviewers against inclusion and exclusion criteria described in the “Detailed Methods” section of eAppendix 1 in Supplement 1 and the protocol.13 Data were extracted from included studies by 1 extractor and verified by a second extractor into the Health Assessment Workspace Collaborative (HAWC) system. Data are publicly available and downloadable (https://hawcproject.org/assessment/405/).
Quality of individual studies, also called risk of bias, was independently evaluated by 2 trained assessors following criteria prespecified in the protocol,13 using the National Toxicology Program’s or Division of Translational Toxicology’s OHAT approach.14 Risk-of-bias questions concerning confounding, exposure characterization, and outcome assessment were considered key. If not addressed appropriately, these questions were thought to have the greatest potential impact on the results.13 The remaining risk-of-bias questions were used to identify other concerns that may indicate serious risk-of-bias issues (eg, selection bias, inappropriate statistical analysis). No study was excluded from the meta-analysis based on concerns for risk of bias; however, subgroup analyses were conducted with and without high risk-of-bias studies (ie, studies rated probably high risk of bias for ?2 key risk-of-bias questions or definitely high risk of bias for any single question) to assess their potential impact, in terms of magnitude and direction of bias, on the results. Ratings and justification are available in HAWC (https://hawcproject.org/assessment/405/).
We conducted the following analyses, planned a priori in the protocol: (1) mean-effects meta-analysis, (2) dose-response mean-effects meta-analysis, and (3) regression slopes meta-analysis (detailed methods are provided in the “Detailed Methods” section of eAppendix 1 in Supplement 1).
The mean-effects meta-analysis included studies that reported mean IQ scores and group-level exposures for at least 1 exposed group and 1 reference group. The effect estimates were standardized mean differences (SMDs) for heteroscedastic population variances.15–17 SMDs were calculated from the difference in mean IQ scores between an exposed group and a reference group. If an individual study reported mean IQ scores for multiple exposure groups, the highest exposure group was considered the exposed group and the lowest exposure group was considered the reference group. A sensitivity analysis was performed to evaluate the impact of all exposure groups combined compared with a reference group. Pooled SMDs and 95% CIs were estimated using random-effects models. To determine whether the data support an exposure-response association, we conducted a dose-response mean-effects meta-analysis that included studies from the mean-effects meta-analysis and used a 1-step approach as described in the protocol.13,18–20 A pooled dose-response curve was estimated using a restricted maximum likelihood estimation method. Potential nonlinear associations were examined using quadratic terms and restricted cubic splines. Model comparison was based on the maximum likelihood Akaike information criterion (AIC).21 To examine associations at lower fluoride levels, subgroup analyses were restricted to 0 to less than 4 mg/L (comparable to EPA’s enforceable drinking water standard for fluoride of ?4 mg/L), 0 to less than 2 mg/L (comparable to EPA’s nonenforceable standard for fluoride in drinking water of ?2 mg/L), and 0 to less than 1.5 mg/L (comparable to WHO’s guideline for fluoride in drinking water of ?1.5 mg/L).4
The regression slopes meta-analysis included studies that reported regression slopes to estimate associations between individual-level fluoride exposures and children’s IQ. Data from individual studies were pooled using a random-effects model.22
Heterogeneity was assessed by Cochran Q test23 and the I2 statistic.24 Subgroup analyses stratified studies by risk of bias (high or low), study location (country), outcome assessment, exposure matrix (eg, urine, water), sex, and age to further investigate sources of heterogeneity. An analysis stratified by prenatal or postnatal exposure was suggested post hoc. Potential publication bias was assessed with funnel plots and Egger tests.25–27 If publication bias was present, trim-and-fill methods28,29 were used to estimate the number of hypothetical “missing” studies and predict the impact of the missing studies on the pooled effect estimate.
Statistical analyses were performed using Stata version 17.0 statistical software (StataCorp LLC).30 The combine, meta esize, meta set, meta summarize, drmeta, meta funnel, meta bias, meta trimfill, and metareg packages were used.31
A total of 74 publications (64 cross-sectional studies and 10 prospective cohort studies) met the inclusion criteria, with 65 included in the primary analyses and an additional 9 included in sensitivity analyses (eFigure 1B in Supplement 1; see eTable 2 in Supplement 1 for excluded publications). Characteristics of the 74 publications and the study-specific effect estimates used in the meta-analyses are shown in eTable 1 in Supplement 1. Most studies were conducted in China (n?=?45); other locations included Canada (n?=?3), Denmark (n?=?1), India (n?=?12), Iran (n?=?4), Mexico (n?=?4), New Zealand (n?=?1), Pakistan (n?=?2), Spain (n?=?1), and Taiwan (n?=?1). No studies were conducted in the United States. Of these, 59 publications reported mean IQ scores for group-level exposures10,11,32–95 and 19 reported regression slopes for individual-level exposures based on urinary or water fluoride concentrations and fluoride intake.9–11,32–38,96–104 Additional details on study characteristics are provided in the “Results” section of eAppendix 1 in Supplement 1. Sixty-four studies reported inverse associations between fluoride exposure measures and children’s IQ. Fifty-two studies were rated high risk of bias. Twenty-two studies were rated low risk of bias, with 13 rated low risk of bias across all 7 risk-of-bias domains and 9 rated low risk of bias in 6 domains and probably high risk of bias in no more than 1 domain. Results from risk-of-bias evaluations are presented in eFigure 2 in Supplement 1. Interactive versions of the figures and risk-of-bias evaluations are available in HAWC (links provided in the “Results” section of eAppendix 1 in Supplement 1). Further details and justification about low risk-of-bias studies are presented in eAppendix 2 in Supplement 1.
The meta-analysis of 59 studies (47 high risk of bias, 12 low risk of bias; n?=?20?932 children) that provided mean IQ scores showed that, when compared with children exposed to lower fluoride levels, children exposed to higher fluoride levels had statistically significantly lower IQ scores (random-effects pooled SMD, ?0.45; 95% CI, ?0.57 to ?0.33; P?<?.001) (Table 1 and Figure 1). There was evidence of high heterogeneity (I2?=?94%; P?<?.001; Table 1) and publication bias (funnel plot and Egger P?<?.001, Begg P?=?.03; eFigures 3 and 4 in Supplement 1). Adjusting for possible publication bias through trim-and-fill analysis supported the statistically significant inverse association after imputation of 2 additional studies to the right side (adjusted SMD, –0.39; 95% CI, ?0.58 to ?0.20) or 17 studies to the left side (adjusted SMD, –0.63; 95% CI, –0.76 to –0.50) (eFigures 5 and 6 in Supplement 1). Fifty-two of the 59 studies (88%) reported an inverse association with SMDs ranging from ?5.34 (95% CI, ?6.34 to ?4.34) to ?0.04 (95% CI, ?0.45 to 0.36) (Figure 1). Seven studies that did not report inverse associations reported SMDs ranging from 0.00 (95% CI, ?0.25 to 0.25) to 0.43 (95% CI, 0.07 to 0.80).10,32,37,39–42 Three studies43–45 lacked clear descriptions of their intelligence assessment methods; however, sensitivity analyses did not reveal substantial changes in the pooled SMD estimate when these studies were excluded or when a study103 that reported the cognitive subset of evaluations using Bayley and McCarthy tests was included (eTable 3 in Supplement 1).
Among the low risk-of-bias studies,10,11,32–35,37,42,47–50 the random-effects pooled SMD was ?0.19 (95% CI, ?0.35 to ?0.04; P?=?.01) with high heterogeneity (I2?=?87%) (Table 1; eFigure 7 in Supplement 1) and no evidence of publication bias (funnel plot and Egger P?=?.56; eFigures 8 and 9 in Supplement 1). Among the high risk-of-bias studies, the random-effects pooled SMD was ?0.52 (95% CI, ?0.68 to ?0.37; P?<?.001) with high heterogeneity (I2?=?94%) (Table 1; eFigure 7 in Supplement 1). There was evidence of publication bias (funnel plot and Egger P?<?.001; eFigures 8 and 9 in Supplement 1); the trim-and-fill analysis had an adjusted pooled SMD of ?0.47 (95% CI, ?0.72 to ?0.23) (eFigures 10 and 11 in Supplement 1).
Subgroup analyses by sex, age, study location, outcome assessment type, and exposure assessment matrix found inverse associations between measures of fluoride exposure and children’s IQ (Table 1; eFigures 12-16 in Supplement 1). The subgroup analyses did not explain a large amount of the overall heterogeneity; however, the degree of heterogeneity was lower for studies restricted to Iran (I2?=?57%), children aged 10 years or older (I2?=?71%), and girls (I2?=?78%) (“Results” section of eAppendix 1 in Supplement 1). The results of the metaregression models indicate that year of publication and mean age of children did not explain a large degree of heterogeneity (“Results” section of eAppendix 1 in Supplement 1).
The dose-response mean-effects meta-analysis included data from 38 studies (eTable 1 in Supplement 1). We excluded studies for which there was evidence that coexposures to arsenic or iodine might be differential.36,41,44,51–54,105 Results from both the analysis of 31 studies with group-level fluoride measurements in drinking water (24 high risk of bias, 7 low risk of bias; n?=?12?487 children) and the analysis of 20 studies with group-level mean urinary fluoride levels (10 high risk of bias, 10 low risk of bias; n?=?9756 children) found that lower children’s IQ scores were associated with increasing levels of fluoride exposure. Based on the linear models, the mean SMD between exposed and reference groups was ?0.15 (95% CI, ?0.20 to ?0.11; P?<?.001) for water fluoride levels and ?0.15 (95% CI, ?0.23 to ?0.07; P?<?.001) for urinary fluoride levels (Table 2; eTable 4 in Supplement 1). Based on the AIC, the best model fit was achieved when restricted cubic spline levels were added to the linear models for drinking water. Given the small difference in AICs between the different models, and considerations of parsimony and ease of interpretability, the linear model results were chosen for the purposes of discussion and are presented in Table 2, although results from all models are presented in eTable 4 in Supplement 1. For fluoride in water, the associations remained inverse when exposed groups were restricted to less than 4 mg/L (16 high risk-of-bias studies, 7 low risk-of-bias studies) or less than 2 mg/L (4 high risk-of-bias studies, 4 low risk-of-bias studies); however, the association was null at less than 1.5 mg/L (4 high risk-of-bias studies, 3 low risk-of-bias studies) (Table 2; eTable 4 in Supplement 1). When we included only studies with low risk of bias, the associations remained inverse at less than 4 mg/L, less than 2 mg/L, and less than 1.5 mg/L fluoride in water, and the linear model was the best fit (Table 2; eTable 4 in Supplement 1). For urinary fluoride, the associations remained inverse when exposed groups were restricted to less than 4 mg/L (4 high risk-of-bias studies, 10 low risk-of-bias studies), less than 2 mg/L (2 high risk-of-bias studies, 4 low risk-of-bias studies), and less than 1.5 mg/L (1 high risk-of bias study, 4 low risk-of-bias studies). When we included only the low risk-of-bias studies, the associations remained inverse at less than 4 mg/L, less than 2 mg/L, and less than 1.5 mg/L for urinary fluoride, and the linear model was the best fit (Table 2; eTable 4 in Supplement 1).
Each of the 19 studies with individual-level fluoride measures (2 high risk-of-bias studies, 17 low risk-of-bias studies) (eTable 1 in Supplement 1) reported urinary fluoride levels,9–11,32–38,96–104 2 reported fluoride intake,32,97 and 2 reported water fluoride levels.32,33 Thirteen studies were included in the primary regression slopes meta-analysis. The 6 remaining studies, including 3 studies96–98 with populations that overlapped with already-included studies32,33,101 and 3 that reported scores based on Bayley assessments,102–104 were included in sensitivity analyses (eTable 5 in Supplement 1).
In the primary regression slopes meta-analysis, the pooled effect estimate from the 13 studies (2 high risk-of-bias studies, 11 low risk-of-bias studies; n?=?4475 children) with individual-level data showed that a 1-mg/L increase in urinary fluoride was associated with a statistically significant decrease in IQ score of 1.63 points (95% CI, ?2.33 to ?0.93; P?<?.001) (Figure 2) with evidence of heterogeneity (I2?=?60%; P?<?.001; Table 3) and no indications of publication bias (eFigures 17 and 18 in Supplement 1). When restricted to low risk-of-bias studies, the decrease in IQ score was 1.14 points (95% CI, ?1.68 to ?0.61; P?<?.001) with evidence of low heterogeneity (I2?=?23%; P?=?.28; Table 3; eFigure 19 in Supplement 1) and a slight indication of publication bias (eFigure 20 in Supplement 1). The trim-and-fill analysis had an adjusted estimate of ?0.78 (95% CI, ?1.33 to ?0.22) (eFigures 21 and 22 in Supplement 1).
Subgroup analyses by risk of bias, sex, country, exposure matrix, outcome assessment type, and prenatal or postnatal exposure found inverse associations between measures of fluoride exposure and children’s IQ (Table 3; eFigures 23-27 in Supplement 1). The sensitivity analyses including reporting scores based on Bayley assessments102–104 showed no substantial changes in the pooled effect estimates (eTable 5 in Supplement 1).
This systematic review and meta-analysis found statistically significant inverse associations between measures of fluoride exposure and children’s IQ. These inverse associations were observed in all 3 sets of meta-analyses: the mean-effects meta-analysis (47 high risk-of-bias studies, 12 low risk-of-bias studies) and dose-response mean-effects meta-analysis (27 high risk-of-bias studies, 11 low risk-of-bias studies) of group-level fluoride exposure, and the regression slopes meta-analysis (2 high risk-of-bias studies, 11 low risk-of-bias studies) of individual-level urinary fluoride. Within each of these meta-analyses, we used prespecified criteria to assess study quality and classify studies into low and high risk of bias. Stratified analyses found similar inverse associations in both study quality strata. Further subgroup analyses by sex, age, timing of exposure, study location, outcome assessment type, and exposure assessment matrix also found inverse associations between fluoride exposure and children’s IQ.
Studies in these meta-analyses included cross-sectional and prospective cohort designs, each study having its own strengths and limitations. Although all studies contribute to our understanding of the overall association, well-designed studies that accurately measure exposure and outcome and adequately account for potential confounding variables are particularly informative. In these meta-analyses, we followed the OHAT approach14 to extract data from each of the published studies and to classify studies into high risk of bias and low risk of bias based on carefully predefined criteria.13 To make our process and decisions transparent, we provide full public access to the extracted data, risk-of-bias ratings, and rationale for those ratings for each individual study. These data can be used by other investigators to evaluate or extend our process and analysis (https://hawcproject.org/assessment/405/).
Studies using group-level exposures were assessed in the mean-effects meta-analysis. An advantage of such studies is that they can, for example, examine communities with different CWS fluoride levels. Although in the United States 40% to 70% of a person’s fluoride intake comes from fluoridated drinking water, there are other sources of fluoride exposure.4 Therefore, relying on CWS levels alone may underestimate an individual’s total fluoride exposure, which may vary considerably among members of a group depending on individual behaviors. Most of the studies in the mean-effects meta-analysis were cross-sectional; however, we have higher confidence in cross-sectional studies when there is evidence of temporality.14 Among the low risk-of-bias cross-sectional studies, most provided information to establish that exposure likely preceded the outcome (eg, only including children who had lived in a community since birth or children who had dental fluorosis).
Studies using individual-level exposures were assessed in the regression slopes meta-analysis, which included 13 studies with urinary fluoride measures, a more precise exposure assessment measure than group-level exposures. Unlike drinking water levels, individual-level urinary fluoride concentrations include all ingested fluoride and are considered a valid estimate of total fluoride exposure.106,107 Fluoride in urine is measured from both single or spot samples and multiple collections. When compared with 24-hour urine samples, spot samples are more prone to the influence of timing of exposure and can be affected by differences in dilution. However, correlations between urinary fluoride concentrations from 24-hour samples and spot samples adjusted for urinary dilution have been described.108 There were several recent North American prospective cohort studies conducted in Canada and Mexico32,96,97,101 that reported maternal urinary fluoride levels comparable to those in the United States.109,110 These studies combined multiple urinary measurements over the course of pregnancy to examine prenatal fluoride exposure during a critical period of brain development. Although the estimated decreases in IQ found in the regression slopes meta-analysis may seem small (1.63 IQ points per 1-mg/L increase in urinary fluoride), research on other neurotoxicants has shown that subtle shifts in IQ at the population level can affect people who fall within the high and low ranges of the population’s IQ distribution.111–115 For context, a 5-point decrease in a population’s IQ would nearly double the number of people classified as intellectually disabled.116
Finally, studies with group-level exposure measurements were used in the dose-response mean-effects meta-analysis of water or urinary fluoride levels. Although we examined 2 nonlinear models, a linear model almost always provided the best fit for both water and urinary data. There was a statistically significant dose-response association between group-level fluoride measures and children’s IQ. In stratified analyses of low risk-of-bias studies, the association remained inverse when exposure was restricted to less than 4 mg/L, less than 2 mg/L, and less than 1.5 mg/L fluoride in water or urine; except for fluoride concentrations less than 1.5 mg/L in water, these results were statistically significant. There was some inconsistency in the best-fit model and a lack of statistical significance at lower levels for water fluoride exposures, leading to uncertainty in the shape of the dose-response curve. This uncertainty is not surprising given the lower number of observations for fluoride concentrations in water (n?=?879 from 3 studies) compared with urinary fluoride concentrations (n?=?4218 from 5 studies). The ability to detect a true effect is reduced at lower exposure levels when exposure contrasts are diminished.117 Although the same cutoffs were used for the water and urine subgroup analyses, fluoride levels in water likely underestimate total fluoride exposures that are better estimated by levels in urine. Variable fluoride exposures from nonwater sources may also decrease the precision of the effect estimates at lower fluoride concentrations in water. In contrast, the best model fit for urinary fluoride concentrations was consistently linear.
Elevated naturally occurring fluoride levels in groundwater (>1.5 mg/L) are prevalent globally and include central Australia, eastern Brazil, sub-Saharan Africa, the southern Arabian Peninsula, south and east Asia, and western North America.118 Although to our knowledge no epidemiological studies addressing fluoride exposure and children’s IQ have been conducted in the United States, significant inequalities in CWS fluoride levels exist by county sociodemographic characteristics, including racial and ethnic composition, especially among Hispanic and Latino communities.119 Of note, there are regions of the United States where CWS and private wells contain natural fluoride concentrations greater than 1.5 mg/L,120 serving more than 2.9 million US residents.119 In addition, the US Geological Survey estimates that 172?000 US residents are served by domestic wells that exceed EPA’s enforceable standard of 4.0 mg/L fluoride in drinking water, and 522?000 are served by domestic wells that exceed EPA’s nonenforceable standard of 2.0 mg/L fluoride in drinking water.1 To reduce risk of moderate-to-severe dental fluorosis, the CDC recommends that parents use an alternative source of water for children aged 8 years or younger and for bottle-fed infants if their primary drinking water contains greater than 2 mg/L of fluoride.121 Currently, there are no recommendations or restrictions on fluoride levels in drinking water based on cognitive neurodevelopmental outcomes.121
To our knowledge, no studies of fluoride exposure and children’s IQ have been performed in the United States, and no nationally representative urinary fluoride levels are available, hindering application of these findings to the US population. Although this meta-analysis was not designed to address the broader public health implications of water fluoridation in the United States, these results may inform future public health risk-benefit assessments of fluoride.
Strengths of this systematic review and meta-analysis include a large body of literature, a predefined systematic search and screening process, risk-of-bias assessment of individual studies, prespecified subgroup analyses, and use of both group-level and individual-level exposure data. The consistency of the inverse associations across the high and low risk-of-bias studies, different intelligence assessment methods, different exposure matrices, different study locations, different analytical approaches, and evidence of a dose-response association strengthen confidence in the conclusion of an overall inverse association between fluoride exposure and children’s IQ. It is notable that there is a diversity of study design factors across studies, which could be described as overall heterogeneity of the body of evidence. In this case, the heterogeneity supports the robustness of the conclusions and is different from heterogeneity in the results, which we did not find in this meta-analysis.
The body of existing literature has limitations in that many of the studies were classified as having high risk of bias. Most of the studies included in the mean-effects and dose-response mean-effects meta-analyses were cross-sectional and had study design and/or methodological limitations. However, the consistency in meta-analytic associations across the high and low risk-of-bias studies and the other subgroup analyses reduced the likelihood that specific biases or potential confounders in individual studies could explain the inverse association between fluoride exposure and children’s IQ.
While several recent studies conclude that fluoride exposures from community water fluoridation are not associated with children’s IQ or other neurodevelopmental outcomes,122–124 the results of the mean-effects meta-analysis were consistent with 6 previous meta-analyses6–8,122,125,126 that reported statistically significant inverse associations between fluoride exposure and children’s IQ scores (see the “Characteristics of Previous Meta-Analyses” section of eAppendix 1 and eTable 6 in Supplement 1).
This meta-analysis found inverse associations and an inverse dose-response association between fluoride exposure and children’s IQ across the multicountry epidemiological literature. There were limited data and uncertainty in the dose-response association between fluoride exposure and children’s IQ when fluoride exposure was estimated by drinking water alone at concentrations less than 1.5 mg/L. Confidence in the associations at lower fluoride levels could be increased by additional prospective cohort studies with individual fluoride exposure measures. These results may inform future comprehensive public health risk-benefit assessments of fluoride.
Accepted for Publication: September 9, 2024.
Published Online: January 6, 2025. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2024.5542
Open Access: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the CC-BY License. © 2025 Taylor KW et al. JAMA Pediatrics.
Concept and design: Taylor, Eftim, Blain, Hartman, Rooney, Bucher.
Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Taylor, Eftim, Sibrizzi, Blain, Magnuson, Hartman, Bucher.
Drafting of the manuscript: Taylor, Eftim, Magnuson, Rooney, Bucher.
Critical review of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Taylor, Eftim, Sibrizzi, Blain, Hartman, Rooney, Bucher.
Statistical analysis: Eftim.
Obtained funding: Rooney, Bucher.
Administrative, technical, or material support: Sibrizzi, Blain, Magnuson, Hartman, Rooney, Bucher.
Supervision: Taylor, Rooney, Bucher.
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported.
Funding/Support: This work was supported by the Intramural Research Program (ES103316, ES103317) at the National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences (NIEHS), National Institutes of Health and was performed for NIEHS under contract GS00Q14OADU417 (order HHSN273201600015U).
Role of the Funder/Sponsor: NIEHS had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; or preparation of the manuscript. NIEHS did have a role in the review approval of the manuscript and the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
Data Sharing Statement: See Supplement 2.
Additional Contributions: We appreciate the helpful input on the draft manuscript from Suril Mehta, DrPH, MPH, Kelly Ferguson, PhD, MPH, Allen Wilcox, MD, PhD, and Alison Motsinger-Reif, PhD (NIEHS). We thank Jonathan Cohen, PhD (ICF), who performed independent verification of the dose-response meta-analysis; Cynthia Lin, PhD, Nathan Lothrop, PhD, Michelle Mendez, PhD, and Alexandra Goldstone, MPH (ICF), for independent quality control of the data; and Jeremy Frye, MSLS (ICF), for conducting literature searches and reference management. No additional compensation was provided.
References
1. US Geological Survey. Fluoride in groundwater: too much of too little of a good thing? Water Resources Mission Area, Colorado Water Science Center; 2020. Accessed May 3, 2020. https://www.usgs.gov/news/comprehensive-assessment-fluoride-groundwater
2. US Department of Health and Human Services Federal Panel on Community Water Fluoridation. U.S. Public Health Service recommendation for fluoride concentration in drinking water for the prevention of dental caries. Public Health Rep. 2015;130(4):318-331. doi:10.1177/003335491513000408 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Recommendations for using fluoride to prevent and control dental caries in the United States. MMWR Recomm Rep. 2001;50(RR-14):1-42.PubMedGoogle Scholar
4. US Environmental Protection Agency. Fluoride: exposure and relative source contribution analysis. US Environmental Protection Agency; 2010. Accessed August 19, 2019. https://www.epa.gov/sdwa/fluoride-risk-assessment-and-relative-source-contribution
5. National Research Council. Fluoride in drinking water: a scientific review of EPA’s standards. National Research Council; 2006. Accessed August 19, 2019. https://nap.nationalacademies.org/catalog/11571/fluoride-in-drinking-water-a-scientific-review-of-epas-standards
6. Choi AL, Sun G, Zhang Y, Grandjean P. Developmental fluoride neurotoxicity: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Environ Health Perspect. 2012;120(10):1362-1368. doi:10.1289/ehp.1104912 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7. Duan Q, Jiao J, Chen X, Wang X. Association between water fluoride and the level of children’s intelligence: a dose-response meta-analysis. Public Health. 2018;154:87-97. doi:10.1016/j.puhe.2017.08.013 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8. Veneri F, Vinceti M, Generali L, et al. Fluoride exposure and cognitive neurodevelopment: systematic review and dose-response meta-analysis. Environ Res. 2023;221:115239. doi:10.1016/j.envres.2023.115239 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9. Grandjean P, Meddis A, Nielsen F, et al. Dose dependence of prenatal fluoride exposure associations with cognitive performance at school age in three prospective studies. Eur J Public Health. 2024;34(1):143-149. Published online October 5, 2023. doi:10.1093/eurpub/ckad170 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10. Lin YY, Hsu WY, Yen CE, Hu SW. Association of dental fluorosis and urinary fluoride with intelligence among schoolchildren. Children (Basel). 2023;10(6):987. doi:10.3390/children10060987 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11. Xia Y, Xu Y, Shi M, et al. Effects of high-water fluoride exposure on IQ levels in school-age children: a cross-sectional study in Jiangsu, China. Expo Health. 2023;16:885-895. doi:10.1007/s12403-023-00597-2 Google ScholarCrossref
12. National Toxicology Program. NTP monograph on the state of the science concerning fluoride exposure and neurodevelopment and cognition: a systematic review. NTP Monogr. 2024;(8):NTP-MGRAPH-8. PubMedGoogle Scholar
13. National Toxicology Program. Protocol for systematic review of effects of fluoride exposure on neurodevelopment. US Dept of Health & Human Services, Public Health Service, National Institutes of Health; 2020. Accessed May 3, 2020. https://ntp.niehs.nih.gov/sites/default/files/ntp/ohat/fluoride/ntpprotocol_revised20200916_508.pdf
14. National Toxicology Program. OHAT Risk of Bias Rating Tool for Human and Animal Studies. US Dept of Health & Human Services, Public Health Service, National Institutes of Health; 2015.
15. Bonett DG. Confidence intervals for standardized linear contrasts of means. Psychol Methods. 2008;13(2):99-109. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.13.2.99 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16. Hedges LV, Olkin I. Statistical Methods for Meta-Analysis. Academic Press; 1985.
17. Rosenthal R. Parametric measures of effect size. In: Cooper H, Hedges LV, eds. The Handbook of Research Synthesis. Russell Sage Foundation; 1994.
18. Crippa A, Thomas I, Orsini N. A pointwise approach to dose-response meta-analysis of aggregated data. Int J Stat Med Res. 2018;7(2):25-32. doi:10.6000/1929-6029.2018.07.02.1 Google ScholarCrossref
19. Crippa A, Discacciati A, Bottai M, Spiegelman D, Orsini N. One-stage dose-response meta-analysis for aggregated data. Stat Methods Med Res. 2019;28(5):1579-1596. doi:10.1177/0962280218773122 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.Orsini N. Weighted mixed-effects dose–response models for tables of correlated contrasts. Stata J. 2021;21(2):320-347. doi:10.1177/1536867X211025798 Google ScholarCrossref
21. Müller S, Scealy JL, Welsh AH. Model selection in linear mixed models. Stat Sci. 2013;28(2):135-167. doi:10.1214/12-STS410 Google ScholarCrossref
22. DerSimonian R, Laird N. Meta-analysis in clinical trials. Control Clin Trials. 1986;7(3):177-188. doi:10.1016/0197-2456(86)90046-2 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23. Cochran WG. The combination of estimates from different experiments. Biometrics. 1954;10(1):101-129. doi:10.2307/3001666 Google ScholarCrossref
24. Higgins JPT, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ. 2003;327(7414):557-560. doi:10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25. Begg CB, Mazumdar M. Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Biometrics. 1994;50(4):1088-1101. doi:10.2307/2533446 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26. Egger M, Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C, eds. Systematic Reviews in Health Care: Meta-Analysis in Context. BMJ Publishing Group; 2008.
27.Egger M, Davey Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ. 1997;315(7109):629-634. doi:10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28. Duval S, Tweedie R. A nonparametric “trim and fill” method of accounting for publication bias in meta-analysis. J Am Stat Assoc. 2000;95(449):89-98. Google Scholar
29. Duval S, Tweedie R. Trim and fill: a simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics. 2000;56(2):455-463. doi:10.1111/j.0006-341X.2000.00455.x PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30. StataCorp. Stata Statistical Software: Release 17.0. StataCorp LLC; 2021.
31. Palmer TM, Sterne JAC, eds. Meta-Analysis in Stata: An Updated Collection From the Stata Journal. 2nd ed. Stata Press; 2016.
32. Green R, Lanphear B, Hornung R, et al. Association between maternal fluoride exposure during pregnancy and IQ scores in offspring in Canada. JAMA Pediatr. 2019;173(10):940-948. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.1729
ArticlePubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33. Yu X, Chen J, Li Y, et al. Threshold effects of moderately excessive fluoride exposure on children’s health: a potential association between dental fluorosis and loss of excellent intelligence. Environ Int. 2018;118:116-124. doi:10.1016/j.envint.2018.05.042 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34. Ding Y, YanhuiGao, Sun H, et al. The relationships between low levels of urine fluoride on children’s intelligence, dental fluorosis in endemic fluorosis areas in Hulunbuir, Inner Mongolia, China. J Hazard Mater. 2011;186(2-3):1942-1946. doi:10.1016/j.jhazmat.2010.12.097 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35. Zhang S, Zhang X, Liu H, et al. Modifying effect of COMT gene polymorphism and a predictive role for proteomics analysis in children’s intelligence in endemic fluorosis area in Tianjin, China. Toxicol Sci. 2015;144(2):238-245. doi:10.1093/toxsci/kfu311 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36. Saeed M, Rehman MYA, Farooqi A, Malik RN. Arsenic and fluoride co-exposure through drinking water and their impacts on intelligence and oxidative stress among rural school-aged children of Lahore and Kasur districts, Pakistan. Environ Geochem Health. 2022;44(11):3929-3951. Published online November 9, 2021. doi:10.1007/s10653-021-01141-4 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref